There is no author summary for this article yet. Authors can add summaries to their articles on ScienceOpen to make them more accessible to a non-specialist audience.
Abstract
Dear editor,
As legislation in the United States and elsewhere changes to allow more people to
use cannabis and its derivatives, it is imperative that we understand the full spectrum
of the effects of its psychoactive compounds in humans. With this in mind, we read
the article by Morgan et al.
1
, which aimed to do just that, with great interest. The authors administered tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC) and cannabidiol (CBD) to participants with varying levels of schizotypy. Participants
then completed a series of tests to assess a range of cognitive domains, as well as
psychotic symptoms. The researchers conclude that participants showed “cognitive impairments”
after THC administration. However, a number of limitations preclude translation of
these findings to the use of cannabis in its relevant milieu.
First, the authors did not test the effects of cannabis itself; participants inhaled
vaporized ethanol solutions containing pure THC, CBD, or a 1:2 THC:CBD mix. Cannabis
is complex,containing dozens of largely uncharacterized psychoactive compounds
2
. One or two of these compounds administered in isolation do not recreate the effect
of cannabis on the brain. Future studies could examine established cannabis strains
with the desired cannabinoid concentrations. This will ensure that we gain knowledge
about cannabinoid effects that represent those that might be experienced by most users
in the general public.
Second, the doses that were administered (8 mg of THC, 16 mg of CBD) do not reflect
those typically used in a single sitting (32.24 mg of THC and 0.56 mg of CBD in an
entire joint
3
). Rather, the authors administered doses that produced “psychotic-like symptoms and
memory impairment”. In other words, they used what would induce the desired effects
rather than what relates to common cannabis use patterns. This renders their findings
irrelevant to most cannabis use. Moreover, many drugs result in negative health outcomes
at high doses, but are relatively harmless or even medicinal at low doses
4
.
Finally, the participants tested did not represent a normal population in several
ways. Participants scored in either the top or bottom quartiles for schizotypy within
a group of cannabis users and smoked either 1–24 or 25+ days per month. These group
subdivisions, especially in terms of the smoking frequency, seem arbitrary. Because
no cannabis-abstinent or non-schizotypal (from a cannabis-abstinent population) control
was examined, it is not possible to rule out participants’ history of cannabis use
itself as a confound. Even with these confounds, the results were not compared with
a normative dataset, despite the use of standardized tests for which this is available
(e.g., Psychotomimetic States Inventory
5
). That these data demonstrate “a pro-psychotic effect of THC” might, at best, be
valid only for this specific population.
We recognize that cannabinoid administration can induce acute psychological changes,
and albeit rare, even psychotic-like states. Our concern is that the paper by Morgan
et al.
1
does not address the effects of cannabinoids in a way that is focused on or relevant
to most cannabis use. In order to do this, participant recruitment and both drug dose
and route of administration should be chosen with cannabis use norms in mind. Without
this, data do not have much significance outside of the narrow experimental setting
in which they were obtained. Moreover, comparison of cognitive scores to normative
data to determine clinical relevance is at the core of clinical neuropsychology. This
must also be adhered to when studying the effects of drug exposure. Not doing so could
not only lead to overinterpretations in the literature but also hinder the ability
of clinicians, policymakers, and the public to engage in scientifically sound discussions
about cannabis and its related effects.
The psychoactive constituent in cannabis, Δ(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), was isolated in the mid-1960s, but the cannabinoid receptors, CB1 and CB2, and the major endogenous cannabinoids (anandamide and 2-arachidonoyl glycerol) were identified only 20 to 25 years later. The cannabinoid system affects both central nervous system (CNS) and peripheral processes. In this review, we have tried to summarize research--with an emphasis on recent publications--on the actions of the endocannabinoid system on anxiety, depression, neurogenesis, reward, cognition, learning, and memory. The effects are at times biphasic--lower doses causing effects opposite to those seen at high doses. Recently, numerous endocannabinoid-like compounds have been identified in the brain. Only a few have been investigated for their CNS activity, and future investigations on their action may throw light on a wide spectrum of brain functions.
The main active ingredient in cannabis, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), can acutely induce psychotic symptoms and impair episodic and working memory. Another major constituent, cannabidiol (CBD), may attenuate these effects. This study aimed to determine the effects of THC and CBD, both alone and in combination on psychotic symptoms and memory function. A randomised, double-blind crossover design compared the effects of (i) placebo, (ii) THC 8 mg, (iii) CBD 16 mg and (iv) THC 8 mg + CBD 16 mg administered by inhalation through a vaporiser. Using an experimental medicine approach to predict treatment sensitivity, we selected 48 cannabis users from the community on the basis of (1) schizotypal personality questionnaire scores (low, high) and (2) frequency of cannabis use (light, heavy). The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS), Psychotomimetic States Inventory (PSI), immediate and delayed prose recall (episodic memory), 1- and 2-back (working memory) were assessed on each day. Results indicated that THC increased overall scores on the PSI, negative symptoms on BPRS, and robustly impaired episodic and working memory. Co-administration of CBD did not attenuate these effects. CBD alone reduced PSI scores in light users only. At a ratio of 2:1, CBD does not attenuate the acute psychotic and memory impairing effects of vaporised THC. Frequent cannabis users may show a blunted anti- psychotic response to CBD, which is of concern due to the high rates of cannabis use disorders in patients with schizophrenia.
The prevailing view is that recreational methamphetamine use causes a broad range of severe cognitive deficits, despite the fact that concerns have been raised about interpretations drawn from the published literature. This article addresses an important gap in our knowledge by providing a critical review of findings from recent research investigating the impact of recreational methamphetamine use on human cognition. Included in the discussion are findings from studies that have assessed the acute and long-term effects of methamphetamine on several domains of cognition, including visuospatial perception, attention, inhibition, working memory, long-term memory, and learning. In addition, relevant neuroimaging data are reviewed in an effort to better understand neural mechanisms underlying methamphetamine-related effects on cognitive functioning. In general, the data on acute effects show that methamphetamine improves cognitive performance in selected domains, that is, visuospatial perception, attention, and inhibition. Regarding long-term effects on cognitive performance and brain-imaging measures, statistically significant differences between methamphetamine users and control participants have been observed on a minority of measures. More importantly, however, the clinical significance of these findings may be limited because cognitive functioning overwhelmingly falls within the normal range when compared against normative data. In spite of these observations, there seems to be a propensity to interpret any cognitive and/or brain difference(s) as a clinically significant abnormality. The implications of this situation are multiple, with consequences for scientific research, substance-abuse treatment, and public policy.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium
or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the
source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were
made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the
article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to
the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license
and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted
use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view
a copy of this license, visit
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
scite shows how a scientific paper has been cited by providing the context of the citation, a classification describing whether it supports, mentions, or contrasts the cited claim, and a label indicating in which section the citation was made.