Despite advances in the assessment and management of suicidal behavior (SB), suicide
remains a leading cause of morbidity and mortality in the United States, and suicide
rates have increased dramatically over the past 30 years.
1
There is ample evidence that suicidal thoughts and behaviors are transdiagnostic phenomena
that can arise even in the absence of other diagnosable mental health disorders.
2
While attempts to elucidate a unified model of SB have identified genetic, neurobiological,
and psychological factors that are associated with increased risk, these risk factors,
or their combination, have only modest statistical and limited clinical utility.
3
Thus, predicting who will engage in SB remains challenging. We assert that, rather
than being a unified construct, SB likely represents a final common pathway of multiple
separate pathological processes. Here, we hypothesize that the pattern of suicidal
thinking helps distinguish at least two of these suicidal subtypes.
Three lines of evidence support the notion that SB results from multiple suicidal
subtypes. First, there is evidence of associations between particular clinical contexts
and features of SB and distinct neurobiological underpinnings. For example, limbic
circuits have been implicated in unplanned SB in late life depression, and serotonin
receptor 1A binding potential in the raphe nuclei has been linked to more lethal SB
in depression.
4,5
Second, contrary to what we would expect with a unified stress-diathesis model, there
appears to be an inconsistent relationship between stressful life events and suicidal
behavior.
6
Third, fine-grained assessments have shown that suicidal thinking varies from fluctuating
to persistent, and that fluctuating and persistent thoughts of suicide are associated
with different risks of SB.
7,8
In light of this, we posit that there are at least two patterns of suicidal thinking:
stress-responsive and non-stress-responsive.
9
We further propose that these patterns have their own risk factors and pathophysiology,
and that they moderate or mediate the relationship between environmental factors,
such as life stressors and access to means, and the timing and lethality of SB. Finally,
we think these patterns fit with existing knowledge about SB while potentially explaining
some of the observed inconsistencies and limited overall predictive value of established
risk factors (Figure 1).
In the stress-responsive pattern of suicidal thoughts, individuals report sudden,
potentially fleeting increases in suicidal thoughts following stressful life events.
We think that individuals with this pattern of suicidal thinking are at higher risk
of less planned SB and that limiting access to means might be particularly important
for them. We hypothesize that the stress-responsive pattern is rooted in childhood
trauma (CT). CT is a risk factor for suicidal behavior across psychiatric diagnoses,
suggesting it plays an independent role in the etiology of suicidal behaviors.
10
CT is also associated with impulsive aggression—the tendency to react to perceived
threats with an intense, emotional, angry response—among adults.
11
Indeed, conceptually, impulsive aggression may serve as a model for the kind of adulthood
response to stress following CT that we propose and has, in fact, also been associated
with SB.
12
Additionally, human and animal studies have identified persistent serotonin system
and hypothalamic–pituitary axis dysregulation following CT, and these same systems
have been implicated in SB.
13,14
Moreover, polymorphisms in serotonin-and cortisol-related genes, and also epigenetic
modifications to serotonin- and cortisol-related genes, have been specifically linked
with SB following CT.
15
However, findings regarding hypothalamic–pituitary axis dysfunction, and especially
the relationship between cortisol response to stress and SB, are mixed, with some
studies showing blunted cortisol response in attempters compared to non-attempters,
and others showing heightened response or no difference.
16,17
The existence of suicidal subtypes might explain these inconsistent findings: our
preliminary data suggest that a potentiated cortisol response to stress is integral
to the stress-responsive pattern of suicidal thoughts, but not to the non-stress-responsive
pattern.
18
In the non-stress-responsive pattern, individuals are more likely to report persistent
thoughts of suicide. We think this pattern is linked to depressive affect and is more
likely to result in more carefully planned SB. A variety of studies support these
connections. In addition to being one of the criteria for diagnosing major depression,
statistical modeling suggests that major depressive episodes play a role in the development
of suicidal thinking.
19,20
Biomarker studies in depression point to a neurobiological basis for more carefully
planned SB, with greater intent to die, including an inverse relationship between
cerebrospinal fluid serotonin metabolite levels, and SB planning and lethality.
21
Studies distinguishing less planned and more planned attempts among depressed patients
have shown that some risk factors previously linked to SB generally are associated
with more planned attempts specifically. These risk factors include more severe and
frequent thoughts of suicide, family history of suicide, and greater suicidal intent.
22
Finally, individuals with histories of more carefully planned attempts demonstrate
better cognitive control than those with prior impulsive attempts, implicating executive
function and reward circuits in the differential pathogenesis of these suicidal subtypes.
23
We have described two suicidal subtypes reflecting different patterns of suicidal
thinking and stress responsivity, but we suspect there are others. Furthermore, these
patterns might overlap or change over time in the same way that psychological and
physiological responses to stress alter over the lifespan.
24
Much work still needs to be done to further operationalize these terms, and to connect
this work with existing knowledge. We advocate for integrated research to further
delineate suicidal subtypes.